Do You Really Know Why You Hate the Incumbent?

We often believe that we understand things better than we do, and we often elect politicians without knowing why we like one and dislike the other.
This post was published on the now-closed HuffPost Contributor platform. Contributors control their own work and posted freely to our site. If you need to flag this entry as abusive, send us an email.

Election season is upon us. The news is filled with stories about primaries and the lineup of candidates for the November elections. If you live in a state with a contested race, then you hear lots of ads. Some of those ads pump up a particular candidate, but many of them also try to tear down the opponent. Politics is a vicious business.

A dangerous undercurrent in the current elections, though, is that the discussion of political issues is superficial and frankly it is often filled with lies. This is not my opinion. The website politifact.com works hard to ferret out truth and lies in statements made by politicians and candidates across the country. Perusing the front page of this website makes clear that there are a lot of statements made by politicians that rate as "Barely true" and even some that rate "Pants on fire" on their Truth-o-meter.

Despite the absence of good information in the environment, many voters have strong opinions. Talk radio on the right and the left is filled with discussions about the dangers of the opposition party. There has also been a strong anti-incumbent bias. Republicans are hoping to see a new President after this year's elections. Democrats have pointed to actions taken by politicians swept into office in the wave of discontent in the 2010 election as a sign that these politicians need to be defeated.

Do these strong opinions reflect a real understanding of the issues?

This question was addressed by Adam Alter, Danny Oppenheimer and Jeffrey Zemla in a September 2010 paper in the Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. They capitalized on a phenomenon called the Illusion of Explanatory Depth, which was first described by Leonid Rozenblit and Frank Keil in a 2002 paper in the journal Cognitive Science.

The Illusion of Explanatory Depth is the observation that people often believe that they understand things better than they do. For example, if you ask people whether they can explain common household objects like zippers, they will say that they can, and then are surprised to discover that there are often big gaps in the explanations they try to generate.

Alter, Oppenheimer, and Zemla asked Democrats and Republicans about their opinions of the positions of a variety of political candidates during the 2008 Presidential Primary. In particular, they wanted to know whether people felt like they understood the candidates' positions on a variety of issues like immigration, health care, and taxes. Most people rated themselves as being fairly knowledgeable about the candidates. But then they were asked to actually describe a candidate's position on an issue that they felt they understood. In fact, people were generally much less good at describing positions than they thought they would be. In fact, after writing this explanation, people rated their actual level of knowledge as much lower than they thought it was earlier.

Interestingly, though, these authors found a way to make people better at judging their own level of knowledge about candidates. Across a number of studies in this paper (most involving explanations of household objects), they found that when people think about objects specifically, they are better able to judge whether they understand how the object works than when they think about the object abstractly. So, your belief about whether you can explain how a zipper works is better if you think specifically about whether you know how the parts of a zipper allow it to function than if you just think generally about whether you understand zippers.

The researchers used a procedure that generally makes people think either specifically about things or abstractly. To get them to think specifically, they asked people to describe how they perform common jobs like washing the dishes. To get them to think abstractly, they asked people to describe why they perform these jobs. Then, they had people rate how well they understood the policy positions of political candidates and how well that matched up with their actual ability to explain those positions.

People who were encouraged to think abstractly showed the Illusion of Explanatory Depth. They were far more confident in their ability to explain a candidate's position than they should have been given their actual ability to explain it. People who were encouraged to think specifically did not show a strong Illusion of Explanatory Depth.

What does this mean for us?

Elections are important. The people who are elected to office will shape our lives. There are big problems out there that need to be solved, and there are legitimate disagreements about how to solve those problems.

Yet, it is clear that we often elect politicians without knowing why we like one and dislike the other. We hear very general words applied to candidates like "socialist," "reactionary," "dangerous" or "conservative," without knowing what those words really mean when applied to a candidate. The results of this study suggest that before we make up our minds about who to vote for, we should think specifically about the candidates. It is good to have passion for the election process, but it is better for that passion to be supported by knowledge.

Popular in the Community

Close

What's Hot