Dolphins Give 'Names' By Copying Each Other's Whistles, Study Suggests

LISTEN: Dolphins Call Each Other By 'Name'
bottle nose dolphin smiling
bottle nose dolphin smiling

By Virginia Morell

Every bottlenose dolphin has its own whistle, a high-pitched, warbly "eeee" that tells the other dolphins that a particular individual is present. Dolphins are excellent vocal mimics, too, able to copy even quirky computer-generated sounds. So, scientists have wondered if dolphins can copy each other's signature whistles—which would be very similar to people saying each others' names. Now, an analysis of whistles recorded from hundreds of wild bottlenose dolphins confirms that they can indeed "name" each other, and suggests why they do so—a discovery that may help researchers translate more of what these brainy marine mammals are squeaking, trilling, and clicking about.

SCROLL DOWN FOR AUDIO

"It's a wonderful study, really solid," says Peter Tyack, a marine mammal biologist at the University of St. Andrews in the United Kingdom who was not involved in this project. "Having the ability to learn another individual's name is … not what most animals do. Monkeys have food calls and calls that identify predators, but these are inherited, not learned sounds." The new work "opens the door to understanding the importance of naming."

Scientists discovered the dolphins' namelike whistles almost 50 years ago. Since then, researchers have shown that infant dolphins learn their individual whistles from their mothers. A 1986 paper by Tyack did show that a pair of captive male dolphins imitated each others' whistles, and in 2000, Vincent Janik, who is also at St. Andrews, succeeded in recording matching calls among 10 wild dolphins "But without more animals, you couldn't draw a conclusion about what was going on," says Richard Connor, a cetacean biologist at the University of Massachusetts, Dartmouth. Why, after all, would the dolphins need to copy another dolphin's whistle?

Unraveling dolphin calls is not easy; they produce their sounds underwater, often in murky conditions, which makes it difficult for scientists to identify which animal is emitting which call. To get around this problem, Stephanie King, a marine biologist also at the University of St. Andrews, and her colleagues analyzed the acoustic recordings of dolphin signature whistles that scientists with the Sarasota Dolphin Research Program made of more than 250 wild bottlenose dolphins briefly captured between 1984 and 2009 in Sarasota Bay, Florida. She also recorded the signature whistles of four captive dolphins and made detailed observations of their behaviors while whistling.

The scientists with the Sarasota program had captured pairs and groups of dolphins and held them separately in nets for 108 minutes on average. During this time, the marine mammals could not see each other, but they could hear each other, and they whistled at a high rate, sometimes giving 5.3 calls per minute. In the wild, too, dolphins use their signature whistle at a high rate, King notes; it is one of the most common sounds they make.

King and her colleagues compared the spectrograms or visual representations of the wild dolphins' whistles and one pair of the captive dolphins, looking for evidence that the dolphin pairs copied each other's sound. They identified whistle-matching in 10 of the 179 dolphins recorded in the Sarasota study, and one of the captive pairs. The males in two pairs had copied each other's whistles, as had eight mother-and-calf pairs, the team reports today in the Proceedings of the Royal Society B. The dolphins did this quickly, too, repeating the other's whistle within 1 second of hearing it. Those dolphins that copied whistles also only imitated the call of their closest social partner. "It means they were calling a specific individual," King says. "They produce the copies when they are separated, which we think shows that they want to reunite with a particular individual." It's what we do if we get separated from our friends at a fair or the mall, she adds. "You don't call out your name; you call the name of your friend. That's how you get back together."

Indeed, dolphins don't copy just anybody's signature whistle; they are very selective in how they use this talent, using it solely to maintain bonds between mother and calf and between allied males, King says. The scientists recorded one pair of males twice, once at the beginning of the project and then 12 years later. Even after all that time, each dolphin "copied the fine details of the other's whistle," King says, an indication of the pair's especially tight bond.

"There's been some evidence before that dolphins are imitating each other's whistles," Connor says. "But this is the first really convincing paper. It's very exciting." "It is fabulous," adds Karl Berg, an ornithologist at the University of California, Berkeley, who specializes in parrot calls. He notes that dolphin results are "strikingly similar to recent work on parrots," which also imitate each other's vocalizations, sometimes as a way of negotiating foraging or roosting decisions.

King found no evidence that the dolphins used their whistle-copying to be aggressive or to deceive other dolphins, as some have hypothesized. The dolphins that imitated another dolphin's whistle did so only to reestablish contact with a specific individual. But in some cases, the dolphins added parts of their own signature whistle to the copy of their friend's, which suggests that they may also be including additional information—which would not surprise either Berg or Connor. "The ability to imitate social companions can lead to more complicated exchanges of information," as well as disagreements, Berg says.

Connor notes that King's study focused on the signature whistles of captured dolphins that were separated from their social partners, and so may not be representative of how wild dolphins more typically use their calls. "How do they use these calls in the wild? Can they add 'help' to someone's signature whistle?" he asks, noting that male dolphins come to each other's aid when battling over females. "Can they add sounds that say they like or dislike someone? If you can say somebody's name, there's no reason why you can't talk behind his back." King is already tackling these questions with a new round of experiments.

In these recordings, you can hear male dolphin A give his signature whistle. Male dolphin B copies A's call in the second recording; B's own signature whistle sounds very different from A's as you can hear in the third recording:

Male A

Male B copying Male A

Male B


ScienceNOW, the daily online news service of the journal Science

Popular in the Community

Close

What's Hot